Original article

Scand J Work Environ Health 2017;43(3):260-268    pdf

https://doi.org/10.5271/sjweh.3627 | Issue date:

Night-shift work is associated with increased pain perception

by Matre D, Knardahl S, Nilsen KB

Objectives The aim of the present study was to determine whether shift workers exhibit increased perception of experimentally induced pain after working night shifts.

Methods The study was a paired cross-over design with two sleep conditions, after at least two nights of habitual sleep and after two consecutive night shifts at work. Fifty-three nurses in rotating shift work participated. The sensitivity to electrically induced pain, heat pain, cold pain, pressure pain and pain inhibition was determined experimentally in each sleep condition. Sleepiness and vigilance were also assessed.

Results Night-shift work (NSW) increased the sensitivity to electrically induced pain and heat pain (P≤0.001). Relative to habitual sleep, electrically induced pain increased by 22.3% and heat pain increased by 26.5%. The sensitivity to cold and pressure pain did not change, changes relative to habitual sleep was <5% (P>0.5). Pain inhibition was 66.9% stronger after NSW versus after habitual sleep (P<0.001). Sleepiness (measured with the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale) increased from 4.1 after habitual sleep to 6.9 after NSW (P<0.001). Vigilance decreased after NSW, measured as a 0.03-second decrease in reaction time (P<0.005).

Conclusions Changes in pain sensitivity after NSW is measurable with clinically relevant effect sizes and may be an important marker for studies comparing the physiological effects of different shift work schedules. Explanations for the differential effect on different pain modalities should be a focus for future studies.

This article refers to the following text of the Journal: 2009;35(3):193-202

The present study sought to determine whether shift workers exhibit altered pain perception after working night shifts. Prospective studies have found that adverse work schedules, including night-shift work (NSW), increase the risk of musculoskeletal pain after several months (1, 2). Shift workers often report lack of restorative sleep, reduced sleep duration, and poor sleep quality (38). Prospective studies indicate that sleep problems are associated with an increased risk for long-lasting musculoskeletal pain (915) and experimental studies suggest that even a relatively modest restriction or disturbance of sleep increases pain sensitivity. This is manifested as larger responses to experimentally induced pain (1621), as increased spontaneous pain (2224), or as altered pain modulation (23, 2529). A recent meta-analysis confirm that sleep restriction increases sensitivity for several types of pain (30). Altered pain modulation may be a potential pathogenic mechanism of musculoskeletal pain disorders among shift workers. Hence, there is a need for knowledge of effects of night shifts on pain system function in the short term.

Experimental studies are typically performed on healthy volunteers, presumably not used to being awake at night. It is possible that shift workers will adapt or habituate to sleep restriction, becoming more robust to the hyperalgesic effect of sleep restriction. Furthermore, working night shifts may affect circadian rhythms differently than experimental sleep restriction. To the authors’ knowledge, there is no experimental study of effects of night-work-induced sleep restriction on pain sensitivity or pain modulation.

Thus, the aim of the present study was to determine whether NSW alters pain perception and pain modulation.

Methods

Subjects

Nurses were recruited by wall postings or brief bulletins at the hospitals’ intranet pages at major hospitals in the Oslo area. Fifty-eight self-reported healthy nurses volunteered for the experiment (figure 1). Five subjects withdrew before the first experimental day, 53 subjects participated in the first sleep condition [mean 31.6, standard deviation (SD) 9.0 years; range 24–57; 41 women] and 40 subjects participated in both sleep conditions. Of the 13 subjects participating in only one sleep condition, 8 dropped out after the habitual sleep condition and 5 dropped out after the night shift condition. Despite an unbalanced dataset, data from all 53 subjects was analyzed since complete case analysis is generally assumed to reduce the robustness of the estimates (31). Due to technical difficulties in some of the procedures, the number of subjects was not the same across outcome measures. A priori power analysis based on a previous study from our laboratory (32) showed that 25 subjects were needed to detect a difference of 1 cm in pain inhibition on a 10 cm visual analog scale (VAS) between the two protocols with a standard deviation of 1.5, assuming a two-sided significance level of 5% and a power of 90%.

Figure 1

Flowchart of study participants. Five subjects withdrew before the pre-test.a Habitual sleep participants (N=27), night shift participants (N=25).b Reasons for dropout after the first sleep condition: personal (N=11), pregnancy (N=2).c Habitual sleep participants (N=21), night-shift work participants (N=19).

SJWEH-43-260-g001.tif

Of the 53 subjects, 39 followed a rotating shift schedule (morning, evening, night), 6 were permanent night-workers and 8 had an unknown shift schedule. Exclusion criteria were: pain with intensity ≥3/10 lasting ≥3 months during the last two years, having psychiatric, neurologic, heart or lung disease (well-regulated asthma allowed), headache of moderate intensity for >2 days per month on average, regular use of over-the-counter analgesics, hypertension (>140/90 mmHg), being pregnant or breast feeding.

All participants received written information and signed an informed consent form. The Norwegian Regional Committee for Medical Research Ethics approved the study (Region South-East B, approval # 2012/199).

Design

The study was a paired cross-over design with block randomization and consisted of two experimental sessions with different sleep condition: after at least two nights with habitual sleep and after two consecutive nights at work, each subject working at his/her regular work place. Except for three subjects having their last night shift three days before the habitual sleep condition, all subjects had ≥4 nights with habitual sleep before the experiment. Most of the subjects (N=29) worked two consecutive nights before the NSW experiment, 13 subjects worked three consecutive nights and 3 subjects worked four consecutive nights. Subjects were instructed to abstain from alcohol the 24 hours prior to the lab experiment. For the NSW condition, the subjects came directly from work to the experiment. All work sites were located <25 km from the research institute, meaning ≤60 minutes with public transport.

Procedure

Subjects were familiarized with the experimental procedure on a pre-test session, two days before the first experimental session. The second experimental session occurred mean 38.1 (SD 39.6) days after the first. The two experimental sessions were identical except for the sleep condition. The same female experimenter tested all participants and was blinded with respect to the sleep condition, giving instructions from a written protocol. The adequacy of the blinding procedure was confirmed by testing on a subset of subjects. The experiment started between 08:00 and 09:00 hours. After ≥5 minutes rest in a seated position, blood pressure was measured three times (Dinamap V100, GE Healthcare) and averaged. Subjective and objective measures of sleepiness were obtained before the experiment. The experimental pain stimuli were delivered in this sequence: pressure pain, 60 electrical pain stimuli, heat pain and finally heat pain in parallel with cold pain. For more details on the experimental pain protocols, see (18, 32).

Sleep, sleepiness and vigilance measurements

At inclusion, habitual daytime sleepiness was measured with the Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS) (33) and a composite measure of sleep disturbance was obtained with the Pittsburg Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) (34). Sleep was monitored via a sleep diary and an accelerometer device worn on the non-dominant ankle (ActiSleep, Actigraph LLC, Pensacola, Florida, USA) 24 hours prior to each experiment. The sleep diary was smartphone-based (paper-based for eight subjects not owning a smartphone). Subjects entered bedtime (lights off), rise time (lights on), and naps in the sleep diary. Actigraphy-based sleep analysis was performed by the Cole-Kripke algorithm (Actilife software v. 6.12.0, Actigraph LLC, Pensacola, Florida, USA). Actigraph recordings were obtained before both experimental sessions in 36 subjects, before one of the sleep conditions in 15 subjects, and, due to technical difficulties, not at all in 2 subjects. In each experimental session, sleepiness was measured subjectively by the Karolinska sleepiness scale (KSS) (35) and vigilance was measured by a computerized version of the 10-minute psychomotor vigilance test (PVT) (custom-written C++ program, National Institute of Occupational Health, Oslo, Norway). We used the mean inverse reaction time from the PVT as a parameter of vigilance, which has been shown to be particularly sensitive to sleep restriction (36).

Experimental pain stimuli

The subjects participated in standardized tests of electrical pinprick pain, contact heat pain, cold pain, and pressure pain. In addition, pain inhibition was tested by the conditioned pain modulation (CPM) paradigm (37).

Electrical stimulation

High-density electrical stimulation was delivered by a constant current stimulator (DS7A and DG2A, Digitimer, Hertfordshire, England) through a platinum pin electrode (cathode, length: 0.2 mm Ø: 0.2 mm) attached to the skin by double-adhesive tape at the volar forearm 10 mm medial to half the distance between the insertion point of the biceps brachii tendon and the distal end of ulna (32). The anode was a conductive Velcro-strap (Alpine Biomed ApS, Skovlunde, Denmark) soaked in an isotonic NaCl solution at the ipsilateral upper arm. Each stimulus consisted of two unipolar 0.5 ms pulses at an interval of 10 ms. Individual pain threshold was determined by a sequence of three ascending series of stimuli, increased by steps of 0.1 mA (from 0 mA). The pain threshold was calculated as the mean of the two last thresholds. Sixty stimuli were presented, equally divided between three intensities, 2 × pain threshold (A), 3 × pain threshold (B) and 4 × pain threshold (C). Pain intensity was rated on a computerized 10 cm visual analogue scale (VAS) with endpoints “not painful” and “worst imaginable pain”.

Heat stimulation

Heat pain was delivered by a 12.5 cm2 peltier thermode (MSA-II, Somedic AB, Solna, Sweden) attached to the volar forearm by a blood pressure cuff (20 mmHg). The stimulus temperature was individualized, defined as the temperature that induced a pain intensity of 6 (“pain6”) on a 0–10 verbal numerical rating scale (NRS, endpoints 0=“no pain”, 10=“worst imaginable pain”). The pain6 temperature was determined in a pre-test session and the same temperature was used for both experimental sessions. Heat pain was rated continuously (VAS, 10 cm, end points “no pain” and “worst imaginable pain”).

Cold stimulation

Cold stimulation was performed by immersion of the hand up to the wrist in 7 °C circulating water (Lauda Alpha RA8, lauda-brinkmann.com), with fingers spread for 2 minutes, and verbal pain scores were obtained at 30-second intervals (0–10 NRS).

Pain inhibition

In the conditioned pain modulation paradigm, the change in pain response to a painful test stimulus (TS) induced by another painful conditioning stimulus (CS) is an indicator of endogenous pain inhibition (32, 37, 38). The 2-minute heat stimulation served as TS and the contralateral 2-minute cold stimulation served as CS. TS was first presented alone. After a 5-minute break, TS + CS was presented simultaneously.

Pressure pain stimulation

Pressure was induced by a handheld pressure algometer (Wagner Force One model FDIX, Wagner Instruments, Greenwich, CT, USA; probe size 1 cm2). A pilot study indicated that pressure pain threshold repeatability was better for the trapezius muscle compared to other muscles tested. The test site was found at 1/3 the distance along an imaginary line connecting the C7 spinous process and acromion. The algometer output was sampled by a computer (custom-written C++ program, National Institute of Occupational Health, Oslo, Norway). The pressure started at 0 kPa and continued until the subject reached a score of 5 cm on a 0–10 cm VAS with end points “no pain” and “worst imaginable pain”. Visual feedback on the applied pressure was used to ensure increasing pressure at a steady rate (aimed at 50 kPa/sec). The procedure was repeated three times. The response to two pressure pain intensities was assessed: the pressure pain threshold (PPT) and the pressure equal to a VAS rating of 5 cm (PP5) on a 0–10 cm VAS. The latter was based on a linear fit between pressure and pain.

Data analysis

Total sleep time, time in bed, number of awakenings, and sleep efficiency were calculated based on the actigraph measurements. Times for lights-off and -on from the last 24 hours were entered manually offline based on the sleep diary. Only main sleeps periods were analyzed by the Cole-Kripke algorithm, whereas daytime napping (seven subjects) was noted in the sleep diary and added to the total sleep time for these subjects.

Electrical pain scores were averaged for each of the three stimulus intensities. Heat pain scores (stored at 1-second intervals) were processed by averaging scores for every 5 seconds, excluding the first 20-second ramp period. This left 20 VAS scores across the 2-minute stimulus period for each subject and condition. The pain inhibitory effect was defined as the difference between the mean VAS rating over the 2-minute TS and TS+CS conditions (VASTS + CS – VASTS). A negative value corresponded to an inhibitory modulatory effect of CS on TS pain ratings. For cold pain, all four pain scores were entered into the statistical model. Similarly, for pressure pain, all three repetitions were entered into the statistical model.

Statistical analysis

Based on visual inspection of the histograms, most of the sleep variables were non-normally distributed. Thus, the paired comparison between sleep conditions was performed with the non-parametric Wilcoxon test for these variables.

Sleep condition (habitual sleep versus NSW) was the primary fixed factor for each pain outcome (electrical pain score, heat pain score, cold pain score, PPT, PP5 and pain inhibition). Given the repeated measures nature of the data, time was added as a fixed factor for heat pain (20 measurements), cold pain (four measurements), PPT and PP5 (three measurements). The pain inhibitory effect was tested by adding the heat pain score during cold pain to the model as a second fixed factor (inhibition), including the sleep × inhibition interaction. All analyses were performed with linear mixed models with an unstructured covariance structure and following the same general procedure. Firstly, which of these factors to include as random slope were determined: sleep condition, stimulus intensity (electrical pain only), and time (heat pain, cold pain, pressure pain). Secondly, sleep condition was entered as a fixed factor. For electrical pain, stimulus intensity was an additional fixed factor. Random intercept for subject was included in all models. The decision of which of the fixed and random factors to keep in each model was based on the Akaike Information Criterion. Maximum likelihood estimation was used for initial test, and restricted maximum likelihood estimation was used for the final models. The fit of each model was tested by visual observation the Q-Q plot of the residuals as an indicator of normality. All statistical analyses were performed in Stata v.13 (StataCorp, College Station, TX, USA).

Results

Sleep disturbance, daytime sleepiness and blood pressure

Habitual Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) scores were available from 24 subjects and ranged from 1–10 (mean 4.9, SD 2.7). Twelve subjects had a PSQI score of ≥5, indicating poor sleep quality. ESS were available from 39 subjects and ranged from 1–16 (mean 7.3, SD 3.6). Seven subjects had a score of ≥11 indicating high daytime sleepiness. Subjects were normotensive (mean systolic blood pressure 114.8 (SD 9.6) mmHg and mean diastolic blood pressure 68.7 (SD 7.7) mmHg).

Sleepiness, reaction time and sleep/wake parameters

Subjective sleepiness increased by 2.8 points (1–9 scale), whereas reaction time increased by 0.03 seconds after NSW versus after habitual sleep (table 1). During the last sleeping period before each experimental session, subjects in the NSW condition slept one hour shorter and had approximately 1.5 fewer awakenings versus the habitual sleep condition, whereas sleep efficiency did not differ between sleep conditions (table 1). Median, minimum, and maximum times for lights-off and -on, as well as time awake before the experiment, are reported in table 1.

Table 1

Descriptive statistics of subjective sleepiness, reaction time, actigraph-based sleep measures, lights-off, lights-on and time awake. Descriptives are made on the full sample, whereas z and P values are from the sample for which both conditions are avaliable. [KSS= Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (1–9); SD=standard deviation]

Habitual sleep (HS) Night-shift work (NSW) za P-valuea


Mean SD Median Min–Max Mean SD Median Min–Max
Subjective sleepiness (KSS) 4.10 1.73 6.87 1.04 -5.3 <0.001
Reaction time (seconds)b 0.38 2.34 0.41 1.80 3.1 0.002
Time-in-bed (hours)c 7.7 1.2 6.6 1.6 3.4 <0.001
Total sleep time (hours)d 7.5 1.3 6.5 1.5 3.1 0.002
Number of awakeningsd 6.1 4.9 4.6 3.6 2.7 0.007
Sleep efficiency (%)d 96.4 2.8 95.8 3.2 -0.3 0.730
Lights-off time (hours) 22.7 22.2–3.7 9.0 4.0– 11.0
Lights-on time (hours) 7.5 4.8– 12.0 15.5 12.0– 19.0
Time awake (hours)e 1.4 0.6– 3.8 17.0 13.3– 21.0

a Wilcoxon signed rank tests.

b Measured by a 10-minute psychomotor vigilance test

c Calculated based on a sleep diary filled out the 24 hours prior to each lab experiment.

d Calculated based on actigraphy measurements from the same 24 hours (Cole-Kripke algorithm) adjusted with lights-off and lights-on times from the sleep ­diary. Self-reported naps were added to the total sleep time.

e Number of hours awake from lights-on until the experiment started.

Pain intensity

Pain after electrical stimulation was scored 22.3 % higher in the NSW condition relative to the habitual sleep condition (table 2). The estimated effect size was 0.51 cm (95% CI 0.24–0.77) on the 0–10 cm VAS (table 3). Pain scores also increased with increasing stimulus intensity. The estimated effect size was 0.91 cm (95% CI 0.70–1.12) on the 0–10 cm VAS for an increase in one stimulus intensity unit (table 3). There was no interaction between sleep condition and stimulus intensity (P>0.25).

Table 2

Descriptive statistics showing pain scores in response to experimental pain stimuli by sleep condition. [SD=standard deviation; VAS=visual analog scale; NRS=numerical rating scale]

Habitual sleep (HS) Night-shift work (NSW) Delta valuea Percent changeb


N Mean SD N Mean SD
Electrical pinprick pain, VASc 40 2.20 1.76 37 2.69 1.74 0.49 22.3
Heat pain (VAS)c 46 4.68 2.84 41 5.92 2.80 1.24 26.5
Cold pain (NRS)d 48 6.75 2.17 45 7.04 2.13 0.29 4.3
Pressure pain threshold (Newton) 46 30.8 14.8 45 30.3 13.3 -0.59 -1.9
Pressure pain at VAS 5/10 (Newton) 46 82.4 60.4 44 79.4 48.2 -3.06 -3.7
Pain inhibition (VAS)c 46 -1.12 1.85 41 -1.88 1.79 -0.75 66.9

a NSW – HS.

b (NSW – HS)/HS×100.

c In cm ranging from 0 = no pain to 10 = worst imaginable pain.

d Ranging from 0 = no pain to 10 = worst imaginable pain.

Table 3

Statistical summary after linear mixed models analyses by experimental pain type. Random intercept for subject was included in all models. Random slope for time was included for heat pain, cold pain, pressure pain and pain modulation. Random slope for sleep was included for electrical pain and cold pain. The coefficient (estimated effect size) is in unit cm for electrical pain, heat pain, cold pain and pain inhibition, and in Newton for the pressure pain measures. [Obs=number of observations per sleep condition. 95% CI=95% confidence interval; VAS=visual analog scale].

Pain type and fixed factor Obs Coefficient 95 % CI P-value
Electrical pinprick pain 231
 Sleep condition 0.51 0.24–0.77 <0.001
 Stimulus intensitya 0.91 0.70–1.12 <0.001
Heat pain 1704
 Sleep condition 1.24 0.52–1.96 0.001
Cold pain 357
 Sleep condition 0.13 -0.56–0.30 0.566
Pressure pain threshold 269
 Sleep condition -0.10 -1.82–2.03 0.917
Pressure pain at VASb 5/10 264
 Sleep condition -0.24 -5.66–5.19 0.932
Pain inhibition 3444
 Sleep condition 0.85 0.28–1.42 0.003
Inhibition -1.13 -1.53– -0.74 <0.001
 Sleep condition × inhibition -0.71 -0.85– -0.57 <0.001

a Stimulus intensity is added as a fixed factor to electrical pain scores since it is responsible for large variation in the outcome.

b In cm ranging from 0 = no pain to 10 = worst imaginable pain.

Heat pain was scored 26.5% higher in the NSW condition relative to the habitual sleep condition (table 2 and solid symbols in figure 2A). The estimated effect size was 1.24 cm (95% CI 0.52–1.96) on the 0–10 cm VAS (table 3).

Figure 2

(A) Heat pain (TS) by sleep and conditioned pain modulation (CPM) conditions. Data are displayed as the median, interquartile range (box), 10–90% confidence intervals (leaf) and 5–95% confidence intervals (dots). There was a main effect of sleep on TS heat pain alone (#:P=0.001), a main effect of CPM alone, TS+CS pain was rated lower than TS pain alone (*:P<0.001) and a sleep × CPM interaction, the CPM effect being stronger after NSW versus after HS ($:P=0.0001). (B) CPM-effect by sleep condition. [CS=conditioning stimulus (cold); HS=habitual sleep; NSW=night shift work. VAS=visual analogue scale.]

SJWEH-43-260-g002.tif

Cold pain sensitivity was scored 4.3% higher in the NSW condition relative to the habitual sleep condition (table 2), corresponding to a non-significant estimated effect size of 0.13 on the 0–10 NRS (table 3).

Pressure pain sensitivity was not significantly associated with sleep condition; the pressure pain threshold decreasing by 1.9 % and pressure pain at VAS 5/10 decreasing 3.7 % (table 2), corresponding to estimated effect sizes of -0.1 N and -0.24 N, respectively (table 3).

Pain modulation

There was a significant pain inhibitory effect of the conditioning cold pain stimulus on heat pain; heat pain was rated lower when given in parallel with cold pain, than when given alone, corresponding to an estimated effect size of -1.13 cm on a 0–10 cm VAS (table 3, open symbols in figure 2A). There was also a significant sleep × CPM interaction, the pain inhibitory effect was -1.12 cm in the habitual sleep condition versus -1.88 cm in the NSW condition (figure 2B), corresponding to an estimated effect size of -0.71 cm on a 0–10 cm VAS (table 3).

Discussion

The present study of shift workers found that two consecutive night shifts increased pain scores to experimental electrical stimulation and heat with clinically relevant effect sizes, whereas experimental cold pain and pressure pain were not significantly changed. The study also showed that NSW increased endogenous pain inhibition.

Effect of night shift work on sleep measures

The present subjective sleepiness of night-shift-induced sleep restriction is comparable to that reported by experimental sleep restriction studies (1719), indicating that experimental sleep restriction studies may be a valuable model to study the effect of night-shift-induced effects on sleep.

Effect of night-shift work on experimental pain

The paired comparison between the response to experimental pain the morning after two nights at work and after two nights with habitual sleep showed that ratings of electrically induced pain increased by 22.3%. This is larger than findings from a previous study from our lab, where a slightly different protocol yielded an increase of 8% (18).

Elevated heat pain sensitivity after NSW (approximately 24%) is comparable to several studies of experimental sleep restriction, using either contact heat or laser stimulation (17, 1921, 32, 3941), although no effect of sleep restriction have been reported on heat-pain tolerance level (42).

An increased pain inhibitory effect following NSW contrasts with several experimental sleep restriction studies (2527, 29) but is in accordance with a previous study from our lab (32) and with another study that points in the direction of increased pain inhibition after sleep restriction (23). One should keep in mind that endogenous pain modulation is not an unambiguous phenomenon, and that test paradigms vary considerably making comparisons across studies challenging (38, 43).

Cold-pain sensitivity after NSW and sleep restriction has been little investigated, but lowered cold pain thresholds have been reported in at least two previous studies (17, 41). The present study found that supra-threshold pain reports were unaffected by NSW. Of note, the present 2-minute cold-water immersion was given in parallel with heat pain in the CPM protocol. Subjects were told to focus on the heat pain rating, which may have rendered the cold pain ratings less accurate.

Our finding of unchanged pressure-pain sensitivity following NSW contrasts with earlier studies of experimental sleep restriction (18, 19, 42). Relative to the previous study from our lab, the present study population were somewhat older. People tend to be less sensitive to experimental pain with increasing age (44). Another difference is the availability for the shift workers to reduce the sleep debt with recovery sleep between the two nights at work. Circadian factors not accounted for in the present study may potentially also affect pressure pain and cold pain differently than electrical and heat pain.

Methodological considerations

Because the number of waking hours before testing differed between sleep conditions, one may argue that the experiments should have been performed after recovery sleep following the second night shift. However, the long waking time cannot fully explain the results. In experimental studies, partial sleep restriction (of the first part of the night) induces hyperalgesia in the same manner as full sleep deprivation (18). In addition, in the partial sleep restriction studies, the time awake has been the same for both the exposure and the control condition. However, we have not been able to control for other factors related to circadian rhythm disruption and this is a weakness of the study. Little documentation exist on variation in pain sensitivity due to circadian phase, but one study found only small differences in heat and cold pain sensitivity (45). The number of pain tests is relatively high and one cannot exclude possible carry-over effects between tests. However, this is not likely to have affected our main finding as the order of the tests was fixed.

The subjects of the present study were adults, representative of the working population and somewhat older than subjects in many experimental sleep restriction studies. There is a possibility that subjective health complaints that typically increase with age interact with the effects of the intervention to be tested. On the other hand, revealing significant effects in such a group increases external validity. Another strength of the present study is the use of a sleep diary verified by actigraphy.

Concluding remarks

To our knowledge, the present study is the first to show that professional shift workers exhibit an increased sensitivity to pain after NSW. Moreover, we report differential effects on different pain modalities, and explaining this should be a focus for future studies. Lowered pain threshold increase the risk for sensitization of pain pathways. Accordingly, the findings may indicate that healthy workers are at increased risk for development of pain complaints after night shift, and workers with existing pain symptoms may be at risk for exaggeration of their symptoms. Studies of how recovery sleep may counteract the deleterious effects night shifts are warranted. Changes in pain sensitivity after night shift work are of clinically relevant effect sizes, and may be important endpoints for studies comparing the physiological effects of different shift work schedules.

Acknowledgement

The authors would like to thank Jorid Thrane Stuenæs for collecting the data, Monica Wigemyr and Anbjørn Ree for contributing with data analysis and Øivind Skare for statistical advice. All the subjects are acknowledged for postponing their sleep pressure and coming to the lab after night shift.

Reference

1 

Eriksen, W, Bruusgaard, D, & Knardahl, S. (2004, May). Work factors as predictors of intense or disabling low back pain;a prospective study of nurses’ aides. Occup Environ Med, 61(5), 398-404, https://doi.org/10.1136/oem.2003.008482.

2 

Trinkoff, AM, Le, R, Geiger-Brown, J, Lipscomb, J, & Lang, G. (2006, Nov). Longitudinal relationship of work hours, mandatory overtime, and on-call to musculoskeletal problems in nurses. Am J Ind Med, 49(11), 964-71, https://doi.org/10.1002/ajim.20330.

3 

Waage, S, Moen, BE, Pallesen, S, Eriksen, HR, Ursin, H, Akerstedt, T, et al. (2009, Apr). Shift work disorder among oil rig workers in the North Sea. Sleep, 32(4), 558-65.

4 

Akerstedt, T, Nordin, M, Alfredsson, L, Westerholm, P, & Kecklund, G. (2010, Jul). Sleep and sleepiness: impact of entering or leaving shiftwork--a prospective study. Chronobiol Int, 27(5), 987-96, https://doi.org/10.3109/07420528.2010.489423.

5 

Son, M, Kong, JO, Koh, SB, Kim, J, & Harma, M. (2008, Dec). Effects of long working hours and the night shift on severe sleepiness among workers with 12-hour shift systems for 5 to 7 consecutive days in the automobile factories of Korea. J Sleep Res, 17(4), 385-94, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00675.x.

6 

Takahashi, M, Iwakiri, K, Sotoyama, M, Higuchi, S, Kiguchi, M, Hirata, M, et al. (2008, Sep). Work schedule differences in sleep problems of nursing home caregivers. Appl Ergon, 39(5), 597-604, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2008.01.003.

7 

Ursin, R, Baste, V, & Moen, BE. (2009, May). Sleep duration and sleep-related problems in different occupations in the Hordaland Health Study. Scand J Work Environ Health, 35(3), 193-202, https://doi.org/10.5271/sjweh.1325.

8 

Vedaa, O, Harris, A, Bjorvatn, B, Waage, S, Sivertsen, B, Tucker, P, et al. (2015, Jul 27). Systematic review of the relationship between quick returns in rotating shift work and health-related outcomes. Ergonomics, 1-14.

9 

Sivertsen, B, Overland, S, Pallesen, S, Bjorvatn, B, Nordhus, IH, Maeland, JG, et al. (2009, Mar). Insomnia and long sleep duration are risk factors for later work disability. The Hordaland Health Study. J Sleep Res, 18(1), 122-8, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00697.x.

10 

Sivertsen, B, Lallukka, T, Petrie, KJ, Steingrimsdottir, OA, Stubhaug, A, & Nielsen, CS. (2015, Aug). Sleep and pain sensitivity in adults. Pain, 156(8), 1433-9, https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000131.

11 

Sivertsen, B, Lallukka, T, Salo, P, Pallesen, S, Hysing, M, Krokstad, S, et al. (2014, Apr). Insomnia as a risk factor for ill health: results from the large population-based prospective HUNT Study in Norway. J Sleep Res, 23(2), 124-32, https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12102.

12 

Canivet, C, Ostergren, PO, Choi, B, Nilsson, P, af Sillen, U, Moghadassi, M, et al. (2008). Sleeping problems as a risk factor for subsequent musculoskeletal pain and the role of job strain: results from a one-year follow-up of the Malmo Shoulder Neck Study Cohort. Int J Behav Med, 15(4), 254-62, https://doi.org/10.1080/10705500802365466.

13 

Kaila-Kangas, L, Kivimaki, M, Harma, M, Riihimaki, H, Luukkonen, R, Kirjonen, J, et al. (2006, Jan 1). Sleep disturbances as predictors of hospitalization for back disorders-a 28-year follow-up of industrial employees. Spine (Phila Pa 1976), 31(1), 51-6, https://doi.org/10.1097/01.brs.0000193902.45315e5.

14 

Mork, PJ, & Nilsen, TIL. (2012). Sleep problems and risk of fibromyalgia: Longitudinal data on an adult female population in Norway. Arthritis Rheum, 64(1), 281-4, https://doi.org/10.1002/art.33346.

15 

Gupta, A, Silman, AJ, Ray, D, Morriss, R, Dickens, C, MacFarlane, GJ, et al. (2007). The role of psychosocial factors in predicting the onset of chronic widespread pain: results from a prospective population-based study. Rheumatology (Oxford, England), 46(4), 666-71, https://doi.org/10.1093/rheumatology/kel363.

16 

Schuh-Hofer, S, Baumgartner, U, & Treede, RD. (2015, Sep). Effect of sleep deprivation on the electrophysiological signature of habituation to noxious laser stimuli. Eur J Pain, 19(8), 1197-209, https://doi.org/10.1002/ejp.698.

17 

Schuh-Hofer, S, Wodarski, R, Pfau, DB, Caspani, O, Magerl, W, Kennedy, JD, et al. (2013). One night of total sleep deprivation promotes a state of generalized hyperalgesia: A surrogate pain model to study the relationship of insomnia and pain. Pain, 154(9), 1613-21.

18 

Matre, D, Hu, L, Viken, LA, Hjelle, IB, Wigemyr, M, Knardahl, S, et al. (2015, Oct 01). Experimental Sleep Restriction Facilitates Pain and Electrically Induced Cortical Responses. Sleep, 38(10), 1607-17, https://doi.org/10.5665/sleep.5058.

19 

Faraut, B, Leger, D, Medkour, T, Dubois, A, Bayon, V, Chennaoui, M, et al. (2015). Napping reverses increased pain sensitivity due to sleep restriction. PloS one, 10(2), e0117425, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117425.

20 

Kundermann, B, Spernal, J, Huber, MT, Krieg, JC, & Lautenbacher, S. (2004, Nov-Dec). Sleep deprivation affects thermal pain thresholds but not somatosensory thresholds in healthy volunteers. Psychosom Med, 66(6), 932-7, https://doi.org/10.1097/01.psy.0000145912.24553.c0.

21 

Tiede, W, Magerl, W, Baumgartner, U, Durrer, B, Ehlert, U, & Treede, RD. (2010, Jan). Sleep restriction attenuates amplitudes and attentional modulation of pain-related evoked potentials, but augments pain ratings in healthy volunteers. Pain, 148(1), 36-42, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2009.08.029.

22 

Haack, M, Lee, E, Cohen, DA, & Mullington, JM. (2009, Sep). Activation of the prostaglandin system in response to sleep loss in healthy humans: potential mediator of increased spontaneous pain. Pain, 145(1-2), 136-41, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2009.05.029.

23 

Smith, MT, Edwards, RR, McCann, UD, & Haythornthwaite, JA. (2007, Apr). The effects of sleep deprivation on pain inhibition and spontaneous pain in women. Sleep, 30(4), 494-505, https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/30.4.494.

24 

Lentz, MJ, Landis, CA, Rothermel, J, & Shaver, JL. (1999, Jul). Effects of selective slow wave sleep disruption on musculoskeletal pain and fatigue in middle aged women. J Rheumatol, 26(7), 1586-92.

25 

Haack, M, Scott-Sutherland, J, Santangelo, G, Simpson, NS, Sethna, N, & Mullington, JM. (2012, Apr). Pain sensitivity and modulation in primary insomnia. Eur J Pain, 16(4), 522-33, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpain.2011.07.007.

26 

Edwards, RR, Grace, E, Peterson, S, Klick, B, Haythornthwaite, JA, & Smith, MT. (2009, Nov). Sleep continuity and architecture: associations with pain-inhibitory processes in patients with temporomandibular joint disorder. Eur J Pain, 13(10), 1043-7, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpain.2008.12.007.

27 

Campbell, CM, Bounds, SC, Simango, MB, Witmer, KR, Campbell, JN, Edwards, RR, et al. (2011, Jul). Self-reported sleep duration associated with distraction analgesia, hyperemia, and secondary hyperalgesia in the heat-capsaicin nociceptive model. Eur J Pain, 15(6), 561-7, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpain.2010.11.014.

28 

Matre, D, Andersen, MR, Knardahl, S, & Nilsen, KB. (2016, Mar). Conditioned pain modulation is not decreased after partial sleep restriction. Eur J Pain, 20(3), 408-16, https://doi.org/10.1002/ejp.741.

29 

Edwards, RR, Quartana, PJ, Allen, RP, Greenbaum, S, Earley, CJ, & Smith, MT. (2011, Jun). Alterations in pain responses in treated and untreated patients with restless legs syndrome: Associations with sleep disruption. Sleep Med, 12(6), 603-9, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2010.09.018.

30 

Schrimpf, M, Liegl, G, Boeckle, M, Leitner, A, Geisler, P, & Pieh, C. (2015, Nov). The effect of sleep deprivation on pain perception in healthy subjects: a meta-analysis. Sleep Med, 16(11), 1313-20, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2015.07.022.

31 

Fitzmaurice, GM, Laird, NM, & Ware, JH. (2011). Applied Longitudinal Analysis (Second ed). Hoboken, New Jersey: Wiley.

32 

Matre, D, Andersen, MR, Knardahl, S, & Nilsen, KB. (2015, Jun 24). Conditioned pain modulation is not decreased after partial sleep restriction. Eur J Pain, 20(3), 408-16, https://doi.org/10.1002/ejp.741.

33 

Johns, MW. (1991, Dec). A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep, 14(6), 540-5, https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/14.6.540.

34 

Buysse, DJ, Reynolds, CF, 3rd, Monk, TH, Berman, SR, & Kupfer, DJ. (1989, May). The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res, 28(2), 193-213, https://doi.org/10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4.

35 

Akerstedt, T, & Gillberg, M. (1990, May). Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. Int J Neurosci, 52(1-2), 29-37, https://doi.org/10.3109/00207459008994241.

36 

Basner, M, & Dinges, DF. (2011, May). Maximizing sensitivity of the psychomotor vigilance test (PVT) to sleep loss. Sleep, 34(5), 581-91.

37 

Yarnitsky, D, Arendt-Nielsen, L, Bouhassira, D, Edwards, RR, Fillingim, RB, Granot, M, et al. (2010, Apr). Recommendations on terminology and practice of psychophysical DNIC testing. Eur J Pain, 14(4), 339, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpain.2010.02.004.

38 

Pud, D, Granovsky, Y, & Yarnitsky, D. (2009). The methodology of experimentally induced diffuse noxious inhibitory control (DNIC)-like effect in humans. Pain, 144(1-2), 16-9, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2009.02.015.

39 

Azevedo, E, Manzano, GM, Silva, A, Martins, R, Andersen, ML, & Tufik, S. (2011, Sep). The effects of total and REM sleep deprivation on laser-evoked potential threshold and pain perception. Pain, 152(9), 2052-8, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2011.04.032.

40 

Roehrs, TA, Harris, E, Randall, S, & Roth, T. (2012, Dec). Pain sensitivity and recovery from mild chronic sleep loss. Sleep, 35(12), 1667-72, https://doi.org/10.5665/sleep.2240.

41 

Ødegård, SS, Omland, PM, Nilsen, KB, Stjern, M, Gravdahl, GB, & Sand, T. (2015). The effect of sleep restriction on laser evoked potentials, thermal sensory and pain thresholds and suprathreshold pain in healthy subjects. Clin Neurophysiol, 126(10), 1979-87, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2014.12.011.

42 

Onen, SH, Alloui, A, Gross, A, Eschallier, A, & Dubray, C. (2001, Mar). The effects of total sleep deprivation, selective sleep interruption and sleep recovery on pain tolerance thresholds in healthy subjects. J Sleep Res, 10(1), 35-42, https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2869.2001.00240.x.

43 

Matre, D. (2013). Conditioned pain modulation (CPM) is not one single phenomenon – Large intra-individual differences depend on test stimulus (TS) and several other independent factors. Scand J Pain, 4, 8-9, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sjpain.2012.11.003.

44 

Neziri, AY, Curatolo, M, Nuesch, E, Scaramozzino, P, Andersen, OK, Arendt-Nielsen, L, et al. (2011, May). Factor analysis of responses to thermal, electrical, and mechanical painful stimuli supports the importance of multi-modal pain assessment. Pain, 152(5), 1146-55, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2011.01.047.

45 

Strian, F, Lautenbacher, S, Galfe, G, & Hölzl, R. (1989). Diurnal variations in pain perception and thermal sensitivity. Pain, 36(1), 125-31, https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(89)90120-6.

Notes

[12] Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.